Utente:Toadino2/Roticità in inglese

Da Wikipedia, l'enciclopedia libera.
Vai alla navigazione Vai alla ricerca

La roticità, in lingua inglese, indica se un parlante pronuncia la consonante rotica storica /r/: è una delle distinzioni più importanti su cui si classificano i dialetti[1]. Nelle varietà rotiche, il fono viene pronunciato in tutte le posizioni, mentre in quelle non-rotiche diventa muto se seguito da consonante. Per esempio, in una variante rotica pronuncerà, approssimativamente, hard e butter ˈ e ˈ, mentre in una non-rotica le /r/ cadrebbero, suonando dunque Template:IPAc-en e Template:IPAc-en.

L'inglese scozzese, l'inglese irlandese e quasi tutti i dialetti nordamericani e canadesi sono varietà rotiche in quanto mantenenti tale fono indistintamente; tra quelle non-rotiche, cioè in cui cade il fono prima di consonante, troviamo i dialetti dell'Inghilterra ad eccezione del sud-ovest, le West Midlands del sud, parti ad ovest del Lancashire così come l'inglese australiano, neozelandese e sudafricano, e zone nel Sud degli Stati Uniti e sulla costa orientale.

Queste cadute cominciarono sporadicamente nel parlato informale nel XV secolo, e nel XVII il fono era effettivamente più debole, ma sempre presente. A metà del XVIII secolo era quasi sempre pronunciato, ma veniva occasionalmente eliso, soprattutto dopo vocali aperte. Agli inizi del XIV secolo la pronuncia non-rotica era divenuta comune a Londra e dintorni, ed andava diffondendosi. Già nello stesso periodo, il britannico meridionale standard era totalmente una varietà non-rotica.

History[modifica | modifica wikitesto]

Red areas indicate where rural accents were rhotic in the 1950s. Based on H. Orton et al., Survey of English Dialects (1962–71). Some areas with partial rhoticity (for example parts of the East Riding of Yorkshire) are not shaded on this map.
Red areas are where English dialects of the late 20th century were rhotic. Based on P. Trudgill, The Dialects of England.

The earliest traces of a loss of /r/ in English appear in the early 15th century and occur before coronal consonants, especially /s/, giving modern "ass (buttocks)" (Old English ears, Middle English ers or ars), and "bass (fish)" (OE bærs, ME bars).Errore script: nessun modulo "Footnotes". The main phase of /r/-loss began during the 15th century, and was characterized by sporadic and lexically variable deletion, such as monyng "morning" and cadenall "cardinal".Errore script: nessun modulo "Footnotes". These /r/-less spellings appear throughout the 16th and 17th centuries, but are uncommon and are restricted to private documents, especially ones written by women.Errore script: nessun modulo "Footnotes". No English authorities describe loss of /r/ in the standard language prior to the mid-18th century, and many do not fully accept it until the 1790s.Errore script: nessun modulo "Footnotes".

During the mid-17th century, a number of sources describe /r/ as being weakened but still present.Errore script: nessun modulo "Footnotes". Ben Jonson's English Grammar, published posthumously in 1640, records that /r/ was "sounded firme in the beginning of words, and more liquid in the middle, and ends."Errore script: nessun modulo "Footnotes". Little more is said regarding /r/ until 1740, when Mather Flint, writing in a primer for French learners of English, said: "...dans plusieurs mots, l’r devant une consonne est fort adouci, presque muet & rend un peu longue la voyale qui le precede." ("...in many words r before a consonant is greatly softened, almost mute, and slightly lengthens the preceding vowel.")Errore script: nessun modulo "Footnotes".

By the 1790s, /r/-less pronunciation was becoming common in London and was quickly increasing in use. The 18th century English actor and linguist John Walker uses the spelling ar to indicate the long vowel of aunt in his 1775 rhyming dictionary.[2] In his influential Critical Pronouncing Dictionary and Expositor of the English Language (1791), Walker reported, with a strong tone of disapproval, that "...the r in lard, bard, [...] is pronounced so much in the throat as to be little more than the middle or Italian a, lengthened into baa, baad...."Errore script: nessun modulo "Footnotes". Americans returning to England after the end of the American Revolutionary War reported surprise at the significant changes in fashionable pronunciation.Errore script: nessun modulo "Footnotes". By the early 19th century, the southern British standard was fully transformed into a non-rhotic variety.

The adoption of /r/-less pronunciation as the British prestige standard in the late 18th and early 19th centuries influenced American port cities with close connections to Britain, causing upper-class pronunciation in the 19th century to become non-rhotic in many eastern and southern port cities such as New York City, Boston, Philadelphia, Alexandria, Charleston, and Savannah.Errore script: nessun modulo "Footnotes". Like regional dialects in England, the accents of other areas in America remained rhotic in a display of "linguistic 'lag'" that preserved the original pronunciation of /r/.Errore script: nessun modulo "Footnotes". Non-rhotic pronunciation influenced American prestige speech until the 1860s, when the American Civil War shifted the centers of wealth and political power to areas with fewer cultural connections to the British elite.Errore script: nessun modulo "Footnotes". By 1870, New York City had become America's national center of commerce and entrepreneurship, and its political and economic leaders were increasingly self-made men with little connection to the old colonial elites and British non-rhotic pronunciation.Errore script: nessun modulo "Footnotes". This largely removed the prestige associated with non-rhotic pronunciation in America, such that when the advent of radio and television in the early 20th century established a national standard of American pronunciation, it became a rhotic variety that fully preserves historical /r/.Errore script: nessun modulo "Footnotes".

Modern pronunciation[modifica | modifica wikitesto]

In most non-rhotic accents, if a word ending in written "r" is followed closely by a word beginning with a vowel, the /r/ is pronounced—as in water ice. This phenomenon is referred to as "linking R". Many non-rhotic speakers also insert an epenthetic /r/ between vowels when the first vowel is one that can occur before syllable-final r (drawring for drawing). This so-called "intrusive R" has been stigmatized, but even speakers of so-called Received Pronunciation frequently "intrude" an epenthetic /r/ at word boundaries, especially where one or both vowels is schwa; for example the idea of it becomes the idea-r-of it, Australia and New Zealand becomes Australia-r-and New Zealand, the formerly well-known India-r-Office and "Laura Norder" (Law and Order). The typical alternative used by RP speakers is to insert a glottal stop where an intrusive R would otherwise be placed.[3]

For non-rhotic speakers, what was historically a vowel plus /r/ is now usually realized as a long vowel. This is called compensatory lengthening, lengthening that occurs after the elision of a sound. So in Received Pronunciation (RP) and many other non-rhotic accents card, fern, born are pronounced [kɑːd], [fɜːn], [bɔːn] or similar (actual pronunciations vary from accent to accent). This length may be retained in phrases, so while car pronounced in isolation is [kʰɑː], car owner is [ˈkʰɑːɹəʊnə]. But a final schwa usually remains short, so water in isolation is [wɔːtʰə]. In RP and similar accents the vowels /iː/ and /uː/ (or /ʊ/), when followed by r, become diphthongs ending in schwa, so near is [nɪə] and poor is [pʰʊə], though these have other realizations as well, including monophthongal ones; once again, the pronunciations vary from accent to accent. The same happens to diphthongs followed by R, though these may be considered to end in /ər/ in rhotic speech, and it is the /ər/ that reduces to schwa as usual in non-rhotic speech: tire said in isolation is [tʰaɪə] and sour is [saʊə].[4] For some speakers, some long vowels alternate with a diphthong ending in schwa, so wear may be [wɛə] but wearing [wɛːɹɪŋ].

Distribution[modifica | modifica wikitesto]

The red areas are those where Labov, Ash, and Boberg (2006:48) found some non-rhotic pronunciation among some whites in major cities in the United States. AAVE-influenced non-rhotic pronunciations may be found among working-class and middle-class African-Americans throughout the country.

Rhotic accents include Scottish English, Irish or Hiberno-English, most varieties of North American English, Barbadian English, and Indian English,[5] and Pakistani English.[6]

Non-rhotic accents include most England English, Welsh English, New Zealand English, Australian English, and South African English.

Semi-rhotic accents have also been studied, such as Jamaican English, in which r is pronounced (as in even non-rhotic accents) before vowels, but also in stressed monosyllables or stressed syllables at the ends of words (e.g. in "car" or "dare"); however, it is not pronounced at the end of unstressed syllables (e.g. in "water") or before consonants (e.g. "market").[7]

Variably rhotic accents, in which speakers often sporadically waver between rhoticity and non-rhoticity without any particular rules of context, are also widely documented. Variably rhotic accents include much of Caribbean English, for example, as spoken in Tobago, Guyana, Antigua and Barbuda, and the Bahamas.[8]

Final post-vocalic /r/ in farmer in English rural dialects of the 1950s[9]
GREEN[ə] (non-rhotic)
YELLOW[əʴ] (alveolar)
ORANGE[əʵ] (retroflex)
PINK[əʵː] (retroflex & long)
BLUE[əʶ] (uvular)
VIOLET[ɔʶ] (back & rounded)

England[modifica | modifica wikitesto]

Though most English varieties in England are non-rhotic, rhotic accents are found in the West Country (south and west of a line from near Shrewsbury to around Portsmouth), the Corby area, some of Lancashire (north and west of the centre of Manchester), some parts of Yorkshire and Lincolnshire, and in the areas that border Scotland. The prestige form, however, exerts a steady pressure towards non-rhoticity. Thus the urban speech of Bristol or Southampton is more accurately described as variably rhotic, the degree of rhoticity being reduced as one moves up the class and formality scales.[10]

United States[modifica | modifica wikitesto]

American English is predominantly rhotic today, but at the end of the nineteenth century non-rhotic accents were common throughout much of the Eastern U.S. and through much of the South along the Gulf. This trend reversed during the mid 20th century, in large part due to the influence of television, as well as the increasing political influence of states to the west (the upper MidWest, then California and Texas). Non-rhotic pronunciations have increasingly been seen as foreign with rhotic accents increasingly seen as American.[11]

Today, non-rhoticity in the Southern dialects are found primarily among older speakers, and only in some areas such as central and southern Alabama, Savannah, Georgia, and Norfolk, Virginia,[12] as well as in the Yat accent of New Orleans. The local dialects of eastern New England, especially Boston, Massachusetts, extending into the states of Maine and New Hampshire, are largely non-rhotic, as well as the traditional Rhode Island dialect; however, this feature has recently been receding. The Greater New York City dialect is traditionally non-rhotic, though William Labov more precisely classifies its current form as variably rhotic,[13] with many of its sub-varieties now fully rhotic, such as in northeastern New Jersey.

African American Vernacular English is largely non-rhotic, and in some non-rhotic Southern and AAVE accents, there is no linking r, that is, /r/ at the end of a word is deleted even when the following word starts with a vowel, so that "Mister Adams" is pronounced [mɪstə(ʔ)ˈædəmz].[14] In a few such accents, intervocalic /r/ is deleted before an unstressed syllable even within a word when the following syllable begins with a vowel. In such accents, pronunciations like [kæəˈlaːnə] for Carolina, or [bɛːˈʌp] for "bear up" are heard.[15] This pronunciation also occurs in AAVE.[16]

Typically, even non-rhotic modern American English varieties do pronounce the /r/ in Template:IPAc-en (as in "bird," "work," or "perky"), realizing it, as in most of the U.S., as [ɝ] or [ɚ] listen.

Canada[modifica | modifica wikitesto]

Canadian English is entirely rhotic except for small isolated areas in southwestern New Brunswick, parts of Newfoundland, and the Lunenburg English variety spoken in Lunenburg and Shelburne Counties, Nova Scotia, which may be non-rhotic or variably rhotic.[17]

New Zealand[modifica | modifica wikitesto]

Although New Zealand English is predominantly non-rhotic, Otago and Southland in the far south of New Zealand's South Island are rhotic due to an apparent Scottish influence.

Asia[modifica | modifica wikitesto]

The English spoken in Asia is predominantly rhotic. Rhoticity in Indian English is probably related to the underlying phonotactics of the native Indo-Aryan and Dravidian languages.[5] In the case of the Philippines, this may be explained because the English that is spoken there is heavily influenced by the American dialect. In addition, many East Asians (in China, Japan, Korea, and Taiwan) who have a good command of English generally have rhotic accents because of the influence of American English. This excludes Hong Kong, whose RP English dialect is a result of its almost 150-year-history as a British Crown colony (later British dependent territory). However, many older (and younger) speakers among South and East Asians speak non-rhotic.

Other Asian regions with non-rhotic English are Malaysia, Singapore, and Brunei. A typical Malaysian English would be almost totally non-rhotic due to the nonexistence of rhotic endings in both languages of influence, whereas a more educated Malaysian's English may be non-rhotic due to Standard Malaysian English being based on RP (Received Pronunciation).

A typical teenager's Southeast Asian English would be rhotic, mainly because of prominent influence by American English. Spoken English in Myanmar is non-rhotic, but there are a number of English speakers with a rhotic or partially rhotic pronunciation. Sri Lankan English may be rhotic.

Mergers characteristic of non-rhotic accents[modifica | modifica wikitesto]

Some phonemic mergers are characteristic of non-rhotic accents. These usually include one item that historically contained an R (lost in the non-rhotic accent), and one that never did so. The section below lists mergers in order of approximately decreasing prevalence.

Panda–pander merger

In the terminology of Wells (1982), this consists of the merger of the lexical sets commA and lettER. It is found in all or nearly all non-rhotic accents,[18] and is even present in some accents that are in other respects rhotic, such as those of some speakers in Jamaica and the Bahamas.[18]

Father–farther merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets PALM and START. It is found in the speech of the great majority of non-rhotic speakers, including those of England, Wales, the United States, the Caribbean, Australia, New Zealand and South Africa. It may be absent in some non-rhotic speakers in the Bahamas.[18]

Pawn–porn merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets THOUGHT and NORTH. It is found in most of the same accents as the father–farther merger described above, but is absent from the Bahamas and Guyana.[18]

Caught–court merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets THOUGHT and FORCE. It is found in those non-rhotic accents containing the pawnporn merger that have also undergone the horse–hoarse merger. These include the accents of Southern England, Wales, non-rhotic New York City speakers, Trinidad and the Southern hemisphere. In such accents a three-way merger awe-or-ore/oar results.

Calve–carve merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets BATH and START. It is found in some non-rhotic accents with broad A in words like "bath". It is general in southern England (excluding rhotic speakers), Trinidad, the Bahamas, and the Southern hemisphere. It is a possibility for Welsh, Eastern New England, Jamaican, and Guyanese speakers.

Paw–poor merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets THOUGHT and CURE. It is found in those non-rhotic accents containing the caughtcourt merger that have also undergone the pour–poor merger. Wells lists it unequivocally only for the accent of Trinidad, but it is an option for non-rhotic speakers in England, Australia and New Zealand. Such speakers have a potential four–way merger taw-tor-tore-tour.[19]

Batted–battered merger

This merger is present in non-rhotic accents which have undergone the weak-vowel merger. Such accents include Australian, New Zealand, most South African speech, and some non-rhotic English speech (e.g. Norfolk, Sheffield).

A large number of homophonous pairs involve the syllabic -es and agentive -ers suffixes, such as merges-mergers and bleaches-bleachers. Because there are so many, they are excluded from the list of homophonous pairs below.

Dough–door merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets GOAT and FORCE. It may be found in some southern U.S. non-rhotic speech, some speakers of African American Vernacular English, some speakers in Guyana and some Welsh speech.[18]

Show–sure merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets GOAT and CURE. It may be present in those speakers who have both the dough–door merger described above, and also the pour–poor merger. These include some southern U.S. non-rhotic speakers, some speakers of African American Vernacular English and some speakers in Guyana.[18] It can be seen in the term "Fo Sho", an imitation of "for sure".

Often–orphan merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets CLOTH and NORTH. It may be present in old-fashioned Eastern New England accents,[20] New York City speakers[21] and also in some speakers in Jamaica and Guyana. The merger was also until recently present in the dialects of southern England, including Received Pronunciation—specifically, the phonemic merger of the words often and orphan was a running gag in the Gilbert and Sullivan musical, The Pirates of Penzance.

God–guard merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets LOT and START. It may be present in non-rhotic accents that have undergone the father–bother merger. These may include some New York accents,[23] some southern U.S. accents,[24] and African American Vernacular English.[25]

Shot–short merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets LOT and NORTH. It may be present in some Eastern New England accents.[26][27]

Bud–bird merger[senza fonte]

A merger of /ɜː(r)/ and /ʌ/ occurring for some speakers of Jamaican English making bud and bird homophones as /bʌd/.[28] The conversion of /ɜː/ to [ʌ] or [ə] is also found in places scattered around England and Scotland. Some speakers, mostly rural, in the area from London to Norfolk exhibit this conversion, mainly before voiceless fricatives. This gives pronunciation like first [fʌst] and worse [wʌs]. The word cuss appears to derive from the application of this sound change to the word curse. Similarly, lurve is coined from love.

Oil–earl merger / coil–curl merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets CHOICE and NURSE preconsonantally. It was present in older New York and New Orleans regional accents, but became stigmatized and is sharply recessive in those born since the Second World War.[29] This merger is known for the word soitanly, used often by the Three Stooges comedian Curly Howard as a variant of certainly in comedy shorts of the 1930s and 1940s.

Other mergers

In some accents, syllabification may interact with rhoticity, resulting in homophones where non-rhotic accents have centering diphthongs. Possibilities include Korea–career,[30] Shi'a–sheer, and Maia–mire,[31] while skua may be identical with the second syllable of obscure.[32]

Effect on spelling[modifica | modifica wikitesto]

Template:ChineseText Template:Contains Indic text Template:KoreanText Template:Contains Urdu text Spellings based on non-rhotic pronunciation of dialectal or foreign words can result in mispronunciations if read by rhotic speakers. Examples include:

References[modifica | modifica wikitesto]

Footnotes
  1. ^ Si ricordi che nel mondo anglofono per dialetto si intende principalmente la varietà regionale della lingua.
  2. ^ Labov, Ash, and Boberg (2006): 47.
  3. ^ Wells, Accents of English, 1:224.
  4. ^ Shorter Oxford English Dictionary
  5. ^ a b J. C. Wells, Accents of English 3: Beyond the British Isles, Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press, 1982, p. 629, ISBN 0-521-28541-0.
  6. ^ [1][collegamento interrotto]
  7. ^ Wells, Accents of English, pp. 76, 221
  8. ^ Edgar Schneider, Varieties of English: The Americas and the Caribbean, Walter de Gruyter, 2008, p. 396.
  9. ^ Wakelyn, Martin: "Rural dialects in England", in: Trudgill, Peter (1984): Language in the British Isles, p.77
  10. ^ Peter Trudgill, Language in the British Isles, Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press, 1984, ISBN 9780521284097.
  11. ^ Milla, Robert McColl, English Historical Sociolinguistics, Edinburgh University Press, 2012, pp. 25-26, ISBN 978-0748641819.
  12. ^ Labov, Ash, and Boberg, 2006: pp. 47–48.
  13. ^ Peter Trudgill, Investigations in Sociohistorical Linguistics, Cambridge University Press, 2010.
  14. ^ Gick, Bryan. 1999. A gesture-based account of intrusive consonants in English. Phonology 16: 1, pp. 29–54. (pdf). Accessed November 12, 2010.
  15. ^ Harris 2006: pp. 2–5.
  16. ^ Pollock et al., 1998.
  17. ^ Peter Trudgill, Sociohistorical linguistics and dialect survival: a note on another Nova Scotian enclave, in Magnus Leung, ed. (a cura di), Language Structure and Variation, Stockholm, Almqvist & Wiksell International, 2000, p. 197.
  18. ^ a b c d e f Wells (1982)
  19. ^ Wells, p. 287
  20. ^ Wells, p. 524
  21. ^ Wells (1982), p. 503
  22. ^ Dialectal variant of "horse"
  23. ^ Wells (1982), p. 504
  24. ^ Wells (1982), p. 544
  25. ^ Wells (1982), p. 577
  26. ^ Wells, p. 520
  27. ^ Joey Lee Dillard, Perspectives on American English, The Hague; New York, Walter de Gruyter, 1980, p. 53.
  28. ^ Wells, John C., Accents of English, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, 1982, ISBN 0521229197., pp. 136–37, 203–6, 234, 245–47, 339–40, 400, 419, 443, 576
  29. ^ Wells (1982), pp. 508-509
  30. ^ Wells (1982), p. 225
  31. ^ Clive Upton, Oxford rhyming dictionary, Oxford University Press, 2004, p. 59, ISBN 0-19-280115-5.
  32. ^ Clive Upton, Oxford rhyming dictionary, Oxford University Press, 2004, p. 60, ISBN 0-19-280115-5.
Works cited
  • John Hurt Fisher, British and American, Continuity and Divergence, in The Cambridge History of the English Language, Volume VI: English in North America, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, 2001, pp. 59-85.
  • The Atlas of North American English, Berlin, Mouton de Gruyter, 2006, ISBN 3-11-016746-8.
  • Roger Lass, Phonology and Morphology, in The Cambridge History of the English Language, Volume III: 1476–1776, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, 1999, pp. 56-186, ISBN 0-521-26476-6.
  • Peter Trudgill, Language in the British Isles, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, 1984.
  • J. C. Wells, Accents of English, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, 1982.